Skip to main content
  • Original Article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cognitive functions in Arabic-speaking children with velopharyngeal insufficiency and their impact on speech intelligibility

Abstract

Background

Patients with orofacial cleft are at high risk for neurobehavioral problems including learning disability, impaired language function, psychosocial adjustment issues, and persistently reduced academic achievement. All these factors may be related to decrease intellectual abilities of those patients. The presence of velopharyngeal insufficiency (VPI) leads to affection of speech intelligibility due to atypical consonant productions, abnormal nasal resonance, nasal air emission, compensatory articulatory mechanism, and facial grimace.

Objective

This study aimed at assessing the cognitive functions of patients with (VPI) and their effect on speech intelligibility.

Methodology

Fifty patients with (VPI) were selected from the Outpatient Clinic of the Phoniatric Unit in Assiut University Hospital. All patients were evaluated by protocol of nasality assessment including auditory perceptual assessment of speech, assessment of overall intelligibility of speech, nasoendoscopy, and psychometric evaluation.

Results

The mean intelligence quotient (IQ) of patients with VPI was 75.2 ± 14.5 with a range between 41 and 107. The main defect was present in quantitative potential and then verbal ability followed by visual ability with memory having the highest mean. Patients with repaired cleft palate had the highest score (86.53 ± 9.96), while the least score was reported among those with velopharyngeal disproportion (72.50 ± 9.59). There was a nonsignificant negative correlation between IQ degree and speech unintelligibility (p = 0.82).

Conclusion

About half of the patients with (VPI) have below average mentality. Patients with repaired cleft palate had the highest (IQ) score. Increased (IQ) score was accompanied by decreased speech unintelligibility, although it does not reach the level of significance.

Background

Velopharyngeal insufficiency (VPI) refers to structural defects which result in a gap in the velopharyngeal valve. This is often because the velum is short relative to the position of the posterior pharyngeal wall. Velopharyngeal insufficiency has many causes, including cleft palate, submucous cleft, velopharyngeal disproportion, and acquired defects following various surgical procedures (after adenoidectomy, oral and pharyngeal tumors removal) [1]. VPI implies the presence of hypernasality, inappropriate nasal escape, and decreased air pressure during the production of oral speech sounds with decreased speech intelligibility [2]. The term intelligibility refers to “speech clarity” or the proportion of a speaker’s output that a listener can readily understand. Reduced speech intelligibility leads to misunderstanding, frustration, and loss of interest by communication partners. As a result, communication decreases or remains at a low level [3]. Patients with mild and moderate intellectual disabilities showed distinct difficulties in their speech production that affect both the quality and intelligibility of their verbal output. Their speech is characterized by an overall high error rate and the occurrence of both typical and atypical phonological processes [4].

Children with cleft palate performed more poorly on cognitive-intellectual measures than their peers [4]. A number of studies suggest that the cognitive deficits may be secondary to linguistic deficits. They found that children with cleft palate have lower scores on verbal IQ measures than on performance measures [5,6,7].

Some studies have tried to identify specific patterns of cognitive difficulty in children with clefts. They found problems with visual perceptual skills [7,8,9], while others have shown no deficit in these skills [10, 11]. This study aimed to evaluate the cognitive functions of Arabic-speaking children with velopharyngeal insufficiency and their impact on speech intelligibility.

Methods

Fifty patients aged 3–27 years presenting with velopharyngeal insufficiency due to repaired/unrepaired cleft palate or cleft lip and palate or due to velopharyngeal disproportion were recruited from the Outpatient Clinic of the Phonatric Unit at Assiut University Hospital during the period from April 2019 to April 2020. They were free from syndromic cleft palate, hearing impairment (sensorineural hearing loss), neurological diseases, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), delayed language development, and no history of speech therapy. All patients were evaluated by the following protocol of nasality assessment:

  1. A.

    Patient interview: Data collected from the parents were parent’s education and job, consanguinity, age of the child, analysis of complaint, developmental milestones, illness of early childhood, operative intervention and its effect on regurgitation and speech, previous speech therapy, and subjective impression of the following: hearing, swallowing, mental ability, and scholastic achievement.

  2. B.

    Auditory perceptual assessment (APA) of speech: It was done by three expert phoniatricians after recording the participant’s speech. Speech sample includes reading a standardized text or counting to assess the type of nasality (open, closed or mixed), degree of nasality (mild, moderate, severe), consonant precision, the compensatory articulatory mechanisms (glottal articulation and pharyngealization of fricatives), facial grimace, audible nasal air escape, and overall intelligibility of speech. All these parameters were graded along a 5-point scale in which (0) = normal and (4) = severe affection. Speech intelligibility was assessed according to Subtenly et al. [12]. The rating scale was composed of 5 degrees: (0) = normal for age and sex, (1) = mild difficulty in understanding-repetition not required, (2) = moderate difficulty-repetition required infrequently, (3) = marked difficulty-repetition required frequently, and (4) = unintelligible with repetition.

  3. C.

    Visual assessment of the vocal tract: This includes examining lips, dentition, bite, alveolus, hard and soft palate (if clefted, fistula, scar, and palatal length), and lateral and posterior pharyngeal walls, size of tonsils, size and movement of the tongue, and also examination of nose, ear, and larynx.

  4. D.

    Simple clinical tests: Gutzman’s [a/i] test [13] and Czermak’s [cold mirror] test [14] were performed.

  5. E.

    Flexible nasoendoscopy: (Storz Tele pack X LED-TP100) to assess the movement of the velum, lateral and posterior pharyngeal walls, the movement of each component is given a score of (0–4): (0) = the resting (breathing) position, (2) = half the distance to the corresponding wall, and (4) = the maximum movement reaching and touching the opposite wall. Also, the pattern of closure of the velopharyngeal port, whether coronal, sagittal, circular, or other, the velopharyngeal gap and its size, the presence of adenoid and its size, and the presence of Passavant’s ridge were assessed.

  6. F.

    Language evaluation: By Arabic language test [15] and articulation test [16].

  7. G.

    Psychometric evaluation: By Stanford Binet intelligence quotient 4th edition with its four subtests assessing (verbal ability, visual ability, quantitative potential, and memory) [17].

  8. H.

    Audiologic assessment

  9. I.

    Nasometry: Nasometer 6200 (Kay Elemetrics/PENTAX) was used to measure nasal resonance.

Statistical analysis

Data analysis has been performed using SPSS model 20 IBM SPSS (IBM Corp., Armonk, New York, USA). Categorical data have been offered such as number and percent. Quantitative data with normal distribution are expressed as mean ± standard deviation and range. Student t-test was used to compare quantitative data of two groups, while in the case of more than two groups, ANOVA was used. Quantitative data with abnormal distribution expressed as median (minimum–maximum) and compared by Mann–Whitney U-test was used. The correlation tests were conducted using Spearman’s correlation coefficient to correlate between abnormally distributed data and different parameters. The statistical differences were considered significant when P was lower than 0.05.

Results

Demographic data of studied patients (n = 50)

The mean age (± SD) of enrolled patients was 11.50 ± 4.95 years with a range between 3 and 27 years. Thirty (60%) patients were females, and twenty (40%) were males. In terms of education, seven patients (14%) were preschoolers, 21 (42%) were primary school students, 13 (26%) were preparatory school students, 3 (6%) were secondary school students, 2 (4%) were postsecondary school students, and 4 (8%) were illiterate. Consanguinity was present in 33 patients (66%), and 17 patients (34%) had no consanguinity (Table 1).

Table 1 Demographic data of studied patients

Distribution of the patients according to the cause of VPI

As regards the cause of VPI, cleft palate only (CPO) was the most frequent type in 35 patients (70%), followed by velopharyngeal disproportion 7 (14%), bilateral cleft lip and palate (BCLP) were 5 (10%), and unilateral cleft lip and palate (UCLP) were 3 (6%) patients (Table 2).

Table 2 Distribution of the patients according to the cause of VPI

Auditory perceptual assessment (APA) of patient’s speech

Three patients had mixed nasality, while the other 47 (94%) patients had open nasality. As regards the degree of nasality, it was found that 28 (56%), 19 (38%), and 3 (6%) patients had slight, mild, and moderate degree, respectively. It was found that 14 (28%) patients had no consonant imprecision, while slight, mild, and moderate consonant imprecision was present in 15 (30%), 14 (28%), and 7 (14%) patients, respectively. Ten (20%) patients had glottal compensatory articulation, and 7 (14%) patients had pharyngeal articulation, while 9 (18%) patients had both types of articulation. Audible nasal air emission was absent in only 6 (12%) patients, while it was present in 44 (88%) patients. Facial grimace was detected in 19 (38%) patients. Regarding overall unintelligibility, 21 (42%), 10 (20%), 11 (22%), and 1 (2%) patients had slight, mild, moderate and severe unintelligibility, respectively (Table 3).

Table 3 Auditory perceptual assessment (APA) of patient’s speech

Degree of intelligence quotient and its subclasses among studied patients (n = 50)

The mean IQ (± SD) of all patients was 75.2 ± 14.5 with a range between 41 and 107. As regards IQ subclasses, it was found that 11 (22%), 26 (52%), 11 (22%), and 2 (4%) patients had average, below average, mild MR, and moderate MR, respectively (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Degree of total IQ and its subclasses among studied patients

Subtypes of intelligence quotient among studied patients

The main defect was present in quantitative potential (76.04 ± 13.01) and then verbal ability (79.62 ± 12.11) followed by visual ability (79.88 ± 14.3) with memory having the highest mean (82.02 ± 12.73) (Table 4).

Table 4 Subtypes of intelligence quotient among studied patients

Distribution of subtypes of IQ, nasality degree, speech unintelligibility, and different causes of VPI

There was no significant difference between different causes of VPI and IQ degree. There was no significant difference between different causes of VPI and nasality degree. There was a moderate significant difference between causes of VPI and speech unintelligibility, as patients with BCLP had the highest grade of speech unintelligibility while patients with velopharyngeal disproportion had the least grade of speech unintelligibility (Table 5).

Table 5 Distribution of subtypes of IQ, nasality degree, speech unintelligibility, and different causes of VPI

IQ degree among repaired and unrepaired cases of VPI

It was found that different causes of VPI had highly significant effect on IQ degree (p < 0.001). In general, patients with repaired cleft palate had the highest score (86.53 ± 9.96), followed by unrepaired cleft palate, while the least score was reported among those with velopharyngeal disproportion (72.50 ± 9.59) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

IQ degree among repaired and unrepaired types of VPI

Distribution of IQ degree and speech unintelligibility

It was found that different grades of speech unintelligibility had no significant differences with IQ degree (p = 0.82). But in general, patients with no speech unintelligibility had the highest IQ (86.19 ± 8.89), while the least value was reported among those with moderate unintelligibility (67.10 ± 9.54) (Table 6).

Table 6 Distribution of IQ degree and speech unintelligibility

Correlation matrix between IQ degree, nasality degree, and speech unintelligibility

There was a significant positive correlation between the degree of nasality and speech unintelligibility (p = 0.04). Increased nasality degree is accompanied by increased speech unintelligibility (r = 0.492). There was a nonsignificant negative correlation (p = 0.82) between IQ degree and speech unintelligibility. Also, there was a nonsignificant negative correlation between IQ and nasality degree (p = 0.845) (Table 7).

Table 7 Correlation matrix between IQ degree, nasality degree, and speech unintelligibility

Discussion

Cognitive dysfunction in children with clefts of the lip and palate has been documented for decades [18]. Some studies reported that children with non-syndromic clefts had lower IQs [19, 20] and lower scholastic achievement [20] than that of the general population. In this study, the mean IQ was (75.2 ± 14.5) with a range between 41 and 107. So, there is obvious downgrading in the cognitive function of patients with VPI. This agreed with Persson et al. [21] who found that the group with cleft palate alone had a significantly lower score on the general intellectual capacity test than the control group. This may be explained by some research that found children and adults with cleft lip and palate (CLP) have abnormal brain structure and function. They have smaller brain volumes, with the frontal lobes and certain subcortical nuclei (caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus) being most affected [22]. The brain of adults with isolated cleft palate (ICP) showed normal cerebral volumes, but an abnormality in tissue distribution in which the frontal and parietal lobes were substantially increased in volume compared with normal, and the temporal and occipital lobes were significantly decreased in volume. The cerebellum was also decreased in volume [23]. These differences in brain volume and structure may be related to the cognitive problems in people with orofacial clefts [24].

In this study, patients with UCLP had the least IQ score (70.67 ± 25.93) followed by patients with velopharyngeal disproportion (72.43 ± 8.75) and then patients with CPO (75.74 ± 14.86), while patients with BCLP had the highest IQ score (78 ± 14.3). This disagrees with Nopoulos et al. [25] who found a relationship between severity of clefting and severity of cognitive deficit in which subjects with bilateral CLP (most extensive clefting) were the most severely affected cognitively, while subjects with CPO (least extensive clefting) were the least affected. However, this may be due to the small number of subjects in each clefting group in this study.

Also, Nopoulos et al. [26] showed that adult males with non-syndromic cleft lip and/or palate have a specific pattern of cognitive deficits. Subjects with clefts were found to have general IQ scores below that of their matched controls. Subjects with clefts had specific and significant abnormalities in verbal abilities. On the other hand, motor skills, verbal memory, executive function, and performance on a visuospatial task were not different from their matched control group.

Richman and Eliason stated that overall intellectual functioning is within the average range. However, there were specific cognitive deficits or delays in children with clefts. These deficits appear to affect the verbal abilities and visual-motor function, but the nonverbal and visual-perceptual functions are generally intact [27].

We found that patients with repaired cleft lip and palate had the highest IQ score. This may be attributed to better psychological consequences of early vs. later repair. That is in line with Murray et al. [28] who found lowered cognitive scores in infants having late cleft lip and palatal repair. As the disfigurement caused by unrepaired clefts not only makes these children less appealing to look at but also makes it difficult for parents to interpret infant expressions, early repair helps better face-to-face play and influences the quality of life of infants as they might be accepted better by their families. Also, parents of infants having early cleft lip repair may find it easier to respond to infant social cues [28].

In our study, the different grades of speech unintelligibility had no significant differences with IQ degree. However, patients with no speech unintelligibility had the highest IQ, while the least value was reported among those with moderate unintelligibility. This may be explained by Coppens-Hofman et al. [4] who showed a strong association between the severity of intellectual dysfunction and speech intelligibility. Patients with intellectual disabilities have difficulties in their speech production that affect both the quality and intelligibility of their verbal output. Their speech is characterized by multiple phonological processes. As short-term and long-term verbal memories are both highly involved in speech production, the two systems are impaired in people with intellectual dysfunction. An additional factor to consider as a potential cause of reduced intelligibility is poor auditory feedback due to deficient auditory processing.

There was a significant positive correlation between nasality degree and speech unintelligibility. This may be explained by the fact that hypernasality affects vowel production and causes modification of the spectrum of F1 and F2 such as weakening of formants, decrease in the strength and enhanced bandwidth of F1 and F2, lower in the amplitude of F1 and F2, introduction of pole/zero pairs in the vicinity of F1, and shifts in the formant frequencies. These spectral modifications in the hypernasal speech will have an impact on the articulatory dynamics while producing vowels resulting in vowel centralization and in turn affecting speech intelligibility [29]. This finding agrees with Særvold et al. [30] who found that the presence of hypernasality and reduced intelligibility were clearly associated with speech in cleft palate patients. Children with speech difficulties appear to have higher risk of delayed phonological awareness development, associated literacy problems, and delays in the acquisition of reading skills [31].

Conclusion

Patients with VPI show mild overall cognitive deficit with particular deficit in quantitative potential. These deficits may be due to the same factors that underline the facial cleft-abnormal development. Speech intelligibility is affected by the degree of hypernasality, the degree of cognitive deficit, and the type of VPI. Early intervention for patients with cleft lip and palate prevents the deterioration of speech problem and helps better personality self-confidence and scholastic achievement.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

VPI:

Velopharyngeal insufficiency

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

ADHD:

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

APA:

Auditory perceptual assessment

CPO:

Cleft palate only

UCLP:

Unilateral cleft lip and palate

BCLP:

Bilateral cleft lip and palate

ICP:

Isolated cleft palate

References

  1. Kummer AW (2020) Speech/resonance disorders and velopharyngeal dysfunction. In Kummer, AW. Cleft Palate and Craniofacial Conditions: A Comprehensive Guide to Clinical Management, 4th edn. Jones & Bartlett Learning, Burlington

    Google Scholar 

  2. Witt PD, D’Antonio (1993) Velopharyngeal insufficiency and secondary palatal management. Clin Plastic Surg 20(4):707–21

  3. Shifman A, Finkelstein Y, Nachmani A, Ophir D (2000) Speech-aid prostheses for neurogenic velopharyngeal incompetence. J Prosthet Dent 83(1):99–106

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Coppens-Hofman MC, Terband H, Snik AFM, Maassen BAM (2017) Speech Characteristics and Intelligibility in Adults with Mild and Moderate Intellectual Disabilities. Folia Phoniatr Logop 68(4):175–182. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450548

  5. Lamb MM, Wilson FB, Leeper HA (1973) The intellectual function of cleft palate children compared on the basis of cleft type and sex. Cleft Palate J 10(4):367–377

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Richman LC (1980) Cognitive patterns and learning disabilities in cleft palate children with verbal deficits. J Speech Lang Hear Res 23(2):447–456

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Richman LC, Eliason M (1982) Psychological characteristics of children with cleft lip and palate: intellectual, achievement, behavioral and personality variables. Cleft Palate J 19(4):249–257

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Richman LC, Eliason M (1984) Type of reading disability related to cleft type and neuropsychological patterns. Cleft Palate J 21(1):1–6

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Niermann W, Goyer K (1981) Intelligence in children with cleft lip and palate. Z Plast Chir 5(4):218–227

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Swillen A, Devriendt K, Legius E, Eyskens B, Dumoulin M et al (1997) Intelligence and psychosocial adjustment in velocardiofacial syndrome: a study of 37 children and adolescents with VCFS. J Med Genet 34(6):453–458

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Chen Z, Chen J, Wu J, Zhang X (2001) Factors involved in intelligent development of children with cleft lip and palate. Hua Xi Kou Qiang Yi Xue Za Zhi 19(3):174–177

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Subtelny JD, Van Hattum RJ, Myers BB (1972) Ratings and measures of cleft palate speech. Cleft Palate J 9(1):18–27

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Gutzmann H. Ein Maß für die Nasalität. Arch néerl de Physiologie. 1922;7 T. VII(S 321).

  14. Czermak JN (1965) Gesammelic Schriften. Vienna. vol I: in Luehsinger R, Arnold GE {eds): Voice. Speech and language, Constable, London, p 668

  15. Kotby M, Khairy A, Barakah M, Rifaie N, El-Shobary A, editors (1995) Language testing of Arabic speaking children. Proceedings of the XXIII World Congress of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics. Ain Shams University, Cairo

  16. Kotby MN BS, El-Zomor M, Mohsen E (1985) Polit study for Standardization of an articulation test. Proceedings of the 10th Annual Ain Shams Medical Congress

  17. Melika L (1998) Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale (4th Arabic version). Victor Kiorlos Publishing, Cairo

    Google Scholar 

  18. Conrad AL, Richman L, Nopoulos P, Dailey S (2009) Neuropsychological functioning in children with non-syndromic cleft of the lip and/or palate. Child Neuropsychol 15(5):471–484

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Billig AL, editor (1951) A psychological appraisal of cleft palate patients. Proceedings of the Pennsylvania Academy of Science 29:31

  20. Means BJ (1954) An analysis of certain measures of intelligence and hearing in a sample of the Wisconsin cleft palate population. Cleft Palate Bull 4:4

    Google Scholar 

  21. Persson M, Becker M, Svensson H (2008) General intellectual capacity of young men with cleft lip with or without cleft palate and cleft palate alone. Scand J Plast Reconstr Surg Hand Surg 42(1):14–16

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Munson S, May AM (1955) Are cleft palate persons of sub-normal intelligence? J Educ Res 48(8):617–622

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Spriestersbach DC (1973) Psychosocial aspects of the “cleft palate problem”: vol. U. Iowa Press, I and II

    Google Scholar 

  24. Nopoulos P, Langbehn DR, Canady J, Magnotta V, Richman L (2007) Abnormal brain structure in children with isolated clefts of the lip or palate. Arch Pediatrics Adolescent Med 161(8):753–758

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Nopoulos P, Berg S, Canady J, Richman L, Van Demark D, Andreasen NC (2002) Structural brain abnormalities in adult males with clefts of the lip and/or palate. Genetics Med 4(1):1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Nopoulos P, Berg S, VanDemark D, Richman L, Canady J, Andreasen NC (2002) Cognitive dysfunction in adult males with non-syndromic clefts of the lip and/or palate. Neuropsychologia 40(12):2178–2184

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Richman LC, Eliason MJ (1986) Development inchildren with cleft lip and / or palate: Intellectual, cognitive, personality, and parental factors. Semin Speech Lang 7(3):225–239

  28. Murray L, Hentges F, Hill J, Karpf J, Mistry B, Kreutz M et al (2008) The effect of cleft lip and palate, and the timing of lip repair on mother-infant interactions and infant development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49(2):115–123

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Haque S, Ali MH, Haque AF (2016) Variability of acoustic features of hypernasality and its assessment. Int J Adv Comput Sci Appl 1(7):195–201

    Google Scholar 

  30. Særvold TK, Hide Ø, Feragen KB, Aukner R (2019) Associations between hypernasality, intelligibility, and language and reading skills in 10-year-old children with a palatal cleft. Cleft Palate Craniofac J 56(8):1044–1051

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Stackhouse J, Wells B, Pascoe M, Rees R, editors (2002) From phonological therapy to phonological awareness. Semin Speech Lang 23(1):27–4. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2002-23509

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

No financial support was received for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ZA collects the data of all patients with cleft lip and palate regarding the history, clinical examination, and auditory perceptual assessment and also perform the clinical examination and nasofiberoscopic examination. HA analyzed and interpreted the data regarding the auditory perceptual assessment and nasofiberoscopic examination. EM interpreted all data of patients and performed revision of all section of the research. ZA was a major contributor in writing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hanan A. Mohamed.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Approval of the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine, Assiut University was obtained before initiating the study (IRB number 17100741). Consent to participate: Informed written consent to participate in this study was provided by all participants in the cases above 16 years old and parents /or legal guardians in the cases of children under 16 years old.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aref, EE.M., Hameed, Z.A.A. & Mohamed, H.A. Cognitive functions in Arabic-speaking children with velopharyngeal insufficiency and their impact on speech intelligibility. Egypt J Otolaryngol 39, 135 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-023-00500-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-023-00500-5

Keywords