Skip to main content
  • Original Article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Phonological awareness and reading abilities in elementary-school students with severe-to-profound prelingual hearing loss and unilateral cochlear implants

Abstract

Background

Research findings on the reading outcomes of cochlear implants are inconsistent. Controversy was observed in the reading performance of implanted children, both in relation to hearing aid users and normally hearing children. This cross-sectional study aims to evaluate the phonological awareness and reading skills of school-aged, severe-to-profound, prelingually deafened children using cochlear implants and to identify any associations between these skills and age of implantation, language development, and the chosen side of implantation. Patients who met the selection criteria were assessed using standardized phonological awareness and reading tests, and their scores were compared to normative data.

Results

Patients’ average scores were significantly behind age-matched normative data. Phonological awareness was strongly correlated to reading skills, and both were positively correlated to language abilities. Age at implantation was not correlated to phonological skills yet it was positively correlated to reading scores for patients over 9 years old.

Conclusion

Many cochlear implanted children are still struggling with reading. Phonological development should be considered in the rehabilitation of this group of patients as it was found to be closely associated with reading proficiency.

Introduction

Cochlear implant (CI) has become the standard of care for the majority of children with bilateral severe-to-profound congenital sensorineural hearing loss who receive only a minor benefit from hearing aids. CI was linked to facilitating skills necessary for reading development [1]. It has enhanced the quality of auditory perception, phonological representation of speech, and oral language development [2]. Yet, considerable individual variability in language acquisition, for example, exists in CI children with a full range of outcomes [3, 4].

Compared to deaf children without CI, many studies showed that, due to better spoken language, CI children are better readers [5,6,7]. However, other studies reported that the effect of CI on literacy development was not significantly better than that of hearing aids [8, 9].

Compared to their peers without hearing loss, and despite the fact that they commonly attend mainstream schools, many CI children are still delayed in literacy acquisition [2, 7, 9, 10]. Bouton et al. [11] attributed reading deficits in Cl children to less efficient phonological representations, atypical reading procedures, or both. Nevertheless, there is also evidence that many (up to nearly 70% in some studies) CI children achieve reading scores within or even above the average range for their normally hearing counterparts [1, 8, 12,13,14,15,16]. Marschark et al. [13] attributed the improved reading skills in CI children to improvements in phonological skills or general language abilities.

Magee [17] believed that phonological awareness (PA) may not be a reading prerequisite as it is in children with typical hearing but is rather developed as reading abilities improve. They also added that while some deaf individuals rely on PA, others use orthographic processes alternatively to make judgments of phonological similarity, and such an ability is also acquired through reading experiences.

Other child-related and environmental variables that have been found to be highly predictive of better reading outcomes in CI children were listed by Sarant et al. [15]. These included female gender [12], better cognitive abilities [1, 3, 14, 18], auditory and visual memory [1], speech perception and production skills [14], earlier implantation [1, 8], consistency of CI use [13], higher socioeconomic standard of the family [12, 19], positive family environment, earlier born children [15], spoken language as the predominant mode of communication rather than sign language [18, 20], and oral language competency [1, 19].

Research has primarily focused on the outcomes of auditory performance, speech, and language development in CI children, with notably less focus on reading attainment, despite being a fundamental educational achievement and a crucial skill to academic success. Early identification and intervention for reading abilities is undoubtedly essential to make use of brain plasticity and mitigate the academic delay expected in this population.

The primary objective of this study was to evaluate the PA and reading abilities of school-aged, deaf children with unilateral CI compared to age-matched normative data. The secondary objective was to identify any associations between PA and reading scores and age of implantation, language development, and the chosen side of implantation.

Methods

Study design and duration

This cross-sectional study was carried out in the period between October 2019 and April 2020.

Sampling

Patients were selected conveniently from children with CI visiting a tertiary hospital outpatient clinic. Out of 45 patients assessed for eligibility, 30 were included. The sample size was calculated using the PASS program, setting the alpha error at 5% and power at 80%. Assuming a difference of 10 in the mean scores of the studied tests among cases compared to reference means for normal children, with an estimated standard deviation of 10, produced a sample size of 30 cases.

Participants

Included children were those with a chronological age between 5 and 11 years old to ensure they were developmentally at or above the age of acquisition of skills chosen for assessment, of average intelligence (total IQ ≥ 90) to exclude slow learners and allow a cognitively matched comparison with data of normally developing peers; diagnosed with bilateral severe (70–95 dB) and/or profound (95 or more dB) prelingual sensorineural hearing loss; implanted with a unilateral CI with or without a unilateral hearing aid (bimodal hearing), with a duration of CI between 4 and 6 years; Arabic-speaking, orally educated, and have a well-developed spoken language adequate to understand test instructions; can communicate using ≥ 4-word sentences; and attend mainstream schools to verify exposure to alphabetic learning. Those with syndromic hearing impairment; those with associated (past or present history of) visual problems and developmental, behavioral, or other medical problems; or those using bilateral CIs or bilateral hearing aids were excluded.

Procedures

Participants were subjected to the following protocol to verify selection criteria and collect outcome data: parent interview: to obtain information related to the patient’s personal history, past history, history of hearing impairment, audiological diagnosis and audiometry records, time, duration, and laterality of the CI operation, and rehabilitation details; clinical examination to exclude any syndromic features; formal testing which included the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale-4th edition, Arabic version [21], to provide the total IQ and mental age, language assessment by the Modified Preschool Language Scale-4th edition (PLS–4), Arabic version [22], to determine the child’s language age and abilities, Phonological Awareness Test (PAT) [23], and Arabic Reading Screening Test (ARST) [24]. PAT and ARST both provide normative data for hearing children and consist of 20 sub-tests (listed in the results section), all of which were assessed for all participants, and the sum of the scores is considered the total score. Relevant areas of deficiency were also delineated by contrasting the scores for each sub-test in both PAT and ARST with their 5th percentile values obtained from normally developing children. Scores below these values indicate a delay.

Data analysis

Data was collected through history taking and clinical assessment. Outcome measures were coded, tabulated, and analyzed with IBM SPSS Statistics software (version 27). Quantitative variables were presented as mean, median, range, and standard deviation (SD). Qualitative variables were presented as frequencies and proportions. The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to determine the distribution characteristics of variables and variance homogeneity. Paired data were analyzed using the paired t test and Wilcoxon- sign rank test. Spearman’s correlation was used to analyze linear relationships between quantitative variables. The correlation coefficient, denoted symbolically “r,” defines the strength (magnitude) and direction (positive or negative) of the linear relationship between two numeric variables. r = 0–0.19 is regarded as a very weak correlation, r = 0.2–0.39 is regarded as a weak correlation, r = 0.40–0.59 is regarded as a moderate correlation, r = 0.6–0.79 is regarded as a strong correlation, and r = 0.8–1 is regarded as a very strong correlation. A P value of ≤ 0.05 was accepted as a statistically significant result.

Results

Patient characteristics

The characteristics of the study population are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study participants

PAT and ARST results

Participants’ average total scores in both PAT and ARST were less than age-matched normal average scores (Tables 2 and 3). The distribution of participants, according to their scores in the different sub-tests, when compared to the 5th percentile of normally developing children, is shown in Tables 2 and 3. In PAT, most participants scored below the 5th percentile in all sub-tests. Between 30 and 43% of participants were at the 5th percentile in some sub-tests, namely 2, 4, 7, 11, and 12. Only 10% of participants scored above the 5th percentile in sub-tests 2 and 3. In ARST, most participants scored below the 5th percentile in all sub-tests. Between 20 and 50% of participants were at the 5th percentile in some sub-tests, namely 1, 2, 3, 4, 11, 12, and 13.

Table 2 Results of the Phonological Awareness Test scores
Table 3 Results of the Arabic Reading Screening Test scores

The differences between PAT and ARST total scores and the corresponding age-matched normal average scores were found to be statistically significant by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (P < 0.001). To calculate the difference (%) in total average scores of PAT and ARST between participants and their age-matched normal children (Table 4), the following formula was used: [(age-matched normal average scores − study participants’ scores)/age-matched normal average scores] × 100.

Table 4 The difference between participants’ PAT and ARST total average scores and their age-matched normal average scores

Relationship between PAT and ARST and other variables

A statistically significant strong or very strong positive correlation existed between the PAT and ARST results (Table S1, supplemental material).

When the chronological age at implantation was correlated to PAT and ARST results, the sample was divided into two groups based on their median age: above and below the age of 9. No statistically significant correlation was found between patients’ age at implantation and the PAT total score and most of its sub-tests (Table S2, supplemental material). A statistically significant moderate positive correlation existed between age at implantation and blending syllables into words (r =.56, P = 0.02), isolating initial phonemes (r =.52, P = 0.04), and phoneme-grapheme correspondence (r =.55, P = 0.02) for the group of patients below 9 years old at the time of assessment, and a statistically significant moderate or strong positive correlation existed between age at implantation and isolating final phonemes (r =.58, P = 0.02), isolating middle phonemes (r =.64, P = 0.01), recognizing rhyming words (r =.67, P = 0.008), and phoneme-grapheme correspondence (r =.67, P = 0.009) for the group of patients above 9 years old.

No statistically significant correlation was found between patients’ age at implantation for the group of patients below 9 years old and all of the ARST sub-tests and total score (Table S3, supplemental material). However, for the group older than 9 years old, the age at implantation was statistically significant and positively correlated with almost all sub-tests (mostly a moderate or strong correlation) as well as the total score of the ARST (r =.62, P = 0.02).

A statistically significant moderate positive correlation existed between both PAT and ARST total scores and receptive, expressive, and total language ages (r =.56, .58, .58, P = 0.001 and r =.55, .56, .55, P = 0.002, respectively) (Tables S4 and S5, supplemental material). All PAT sub-tests were statistically significantly positively correlated to participants’ language ages, with the exception of the “segmenting words into onsets and rimes” sub-test and receptive language age, as well as the “deleting final phonemes” sub-test and receptive, expressive, and total language ages. All ARST sub-tests were statistically significantly positively correlated to participants’ language ages, with the exception of the “sentence sequencing task” sub-test and both receptive and total language ages, as well as the “sentence reading comprehension” sub-test and expressive language age, and the “story reading and comprehension” sub-test and receptive language age.

As shown in Table 5, there was no statistically significant difference between both PAT and ARST total scores and the chosen side for implantation.

Table 5 Comparison between right- and left-sided cochlear implantees in terms of their PAT and ARST total scores

Discussion

This study evaluated the phonological skills and reading abilities of CI children. The majority of participants scored below the average normal scores. Both PA and reading skills were strongly correlated to each other, as well as moderately correlated to language development, but not associated with the chosen side of implantation. The age of implantation was correlated to reading development only for older participants.

PAT and ARST results

Since the study involved CI children of different age groups with variable PA and reading profiles, all sub-tests of the PAT and the ARST were tested in all participants, whenever applicable. We used validated tests originally designed to assess normally hearing children, and not simplified versions, to capture the full range of skills, including advanced ones.

Findings are echoing similar results from previous studies where CI children significantly underperformed their normally hearing peers at PA [18, 25, 26] and reading tasks [2, 7, 9, 10]. Better reading performance in normally hearing students was usually attributed to stronger alignment of auditory and visual inputs, more phonological skills, greater speech and language capabilities, wider representation of words, richer vocabulary, and better verbal comprehension [2, 12, 14, 25]. These skills are expected to be less developed in CI children due to the period of auditory deprivation. As shown in the results, for instance, participants have not yet developed age-appropriate language, and accordingly, learning to read changes to an exercise of learning language itself [27]. Furthermore, despite its benefits, their integration into regular schools might have hampered the individualized support that suits their learning pace, and hindered their reading acquisition accordingly.

Conversely, other studies reported higher general levels of reading and/or PA skills in CI children, attaining levels equivalent to hearing peers of their age [13, 16], a finding that was frequently attributed to their enhanced language skills following CIs.

The high standard deviations of participants’ scores in the current study reflect the considerable intra-group variability in performance, as some participants were reading at or above their chronological age’s expected level. This observation is also in accordance with other studies [7, 14, 28].

Relation between PA and reading

PA is the metalinguistic ability to reflect on and/or consciously manipulate the sound structure of language [29] and is usually assessed via tasks that require segmenting, blending, isolating, deleting, or substituting phonological units. It is frequently regarded as a pre-literacy skill [30]. However, the underlying cause of reading deficits in CI children is sometimes debatable, particularly when it comes to the link between PA and reading attainment. While some [31, 32] argued that phonological skills are crucial to good reading outcomes in deaf children, others [33] believed they are less important. Cupples et al. [34] believe that the inconsistent findings reported across studies are probably due to a failure to control other variables that can influence children’s PA and/or reading outcomes.

Despite the fact that phonological representations develop differently in hearing and deaf children who use spoken language, poor phonological development has been found to correlate with poor reading skills in both typically hearing children [29] and the deaf population, including CI users [16, 34, 35]. Such findings are in line with the results of this study.

When Geers and Hayes [3] investigated the role of PA in reading acquisition, they found that high school students with CIs scored better on reading measures than on PA ones. Yet, participants of the current study, who were all elementary students, scored comparably in both PA and reading tasks. It would be interesting to follow up these patients to see how their performance in both tests has differed over the years.

Association of PA and reading with the age of CI

While many studies [6, 12] claim that early implantation is linked to better prognosis in speech and language skills and, subsequently, reading outcomes, some studies [34, 36, 37] report that it has not always guaranteed better outcomes.

Due to the wide age range in the current study, age is predictably an intervening factor. To minimize its effect, we stratified the study group into two: “at or above” and “below” the age of 9 years, which is the median chronological age. The age of receiving CIs was not correlated to PA scores in both age groups, yet it was positively correlated to reading for the older group. The positive correlation between age at implantation and reading competency in the current study might infer that the older deaf patients are implanted, the better reading performance they would develop, but this must be interpreted cautiously. The lack of a negative correlation may be related to the fact that participants received their CIs at an average chronological age of 4 years (or even older for the group of patients ≥ 9 years), which is not young enough to demonstrate the benefits of early implantation because the first 2 years of life are crucial for the auditory input needed for language development. Another explanation may be related to the interference of other confounding variables that influence the reading performance of CI children, e.g., CI technology and device adjustment, efficiency of the rehabilitation program, educational placement, family support, and pre-implant language abilities [19] (e.g., for an older child to receive a CI that is covered by health insurance in Egypt, more advanced language requirements are needed. As shown in Table 1, on average, the group of patients at or above the age of 9 years uttered their first sentence before they received their CI). Thus, the age of implantation should be viewed as just one of several factors influencing prognosis [36]. Moreover, Geers et al. [10] suggested that the effect of age of surgery may be attenuated over time and become non-predictive of outcomes in older CI children.

Association of PA and reading with language abilities

Oral language is fundamental to reading for deaf children. This study showed that the more advanced their language skills were, the higher CI children scored in PA and reading tests. This is in line with Harris et al. [35], who also found a very strong correlation between the language of deaf children (both hearing aid and CI users) and their reading scores. They proposed that good language size is a mediating factor in the relationship between children’s PA and reading skills in both normally hearing and deaf children. Wass et al. [38] also indicated that receptive vocabulary is the most significant predictor of reading comprehension in CI children.

It is necessary to note, though, that no data was available about participants’ language profiles at the time of receiving CI or at any time before the study started. Nonetheless, the age at which they uttered their first words and sentences can provide insight into their language development. Information about the changes in the progression of language, PA, or reading skills over time, how they were compared to normally developing peers, or how they were linked to one another, was not known. Thus, a longitudinal study would be informative to explore how language, PA, and reading skills evolve and are related to one another over the years, and whether the gap between chronological age and reading skills in children with CI widens or reduces with age.

Choosing the ear to be implanted

Yalcinkaya [39] recommended that the left ear be chosen for implantation, as the left hemisphere can undertake the liabilities of the right hemisphere and not vice versa. The author noticed that, in prolonged follow-up, children whose right ears were implanted showed marked improvement in speech perception and language, but they were delayed in advanced reading skills. In the current study, implanting the right or left cochlea showed comparable results in PA and reading tests. Nevertheless, a true comparison of the left versus right ear would necessitate recruiting participants with bilateral implantations or at least controlling other confounding factors that might interfere with outcomes.

Conclusion and future directions

In conclusion, deaf children using CIs are at increased risk of reading disorders. Although CI has enhanced the overall level of reading skills, it has not eliminated such difficulties for all children.

Since PA and reading ability were found to be closely associated, placing more emphasis on phonological growth during rehabilitation is recommended.

A longitudinal study tracking PA and reading skills in CI children from the prereading/preschool period to adulthood is needed to better understand the development of these skills over time.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed in this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CI:

Cochlear implant

PA:

Phonological awareness

PASS:

Power analysis and sample size

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

dB:

Decibel

PAT:

Phonological Awareness Test

ARST:

Arabic Reading Screening Test

SPSS:

Statistical Package for Social Sciences

References

  1. Johnson C, Goswami U (2010) Phonological awareness, vocabulary, and reading in deaf children with cochlear implants. J Speech Lang Hear Res 53(2):237–261. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0139)

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Weisi F, Rezaei M, Rashedi V, Heidari A, Valadbeigi A, Ebrahimi-Pour M (2013) Comparison of reading skills between children with cochlear implants and children with typical hearing in Iran. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 77(8):1317–1321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2013.05.023

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Geers A, Hayes H (2011) Reading, writing, and phonological processing skills of adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear Hear 32:49S–59S. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181fa41fa

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Miller K (2014) A whole new world: technology and its impact on students who are deaf or hard-of-hearing. Themes Sci Technol Educ 8(1):5–16

    Google Scholar 

  5. Thoutenhoofd E (2006) Cochlear implanted pupils in Scottish schools: 4-year school attainment data (2000– 2004). J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 11(2):171–188. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enj029

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Vermeulen AM, van Bon W, Schreuder R, Knoors H, Snik A (2007) Reading comprehension of deaf children with cochlear implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 12(3):283–302. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm017

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Archbold S, Harris M, O’Donoghue G, Nikolopoulos T, White A, Richmond HL (2008) Reading abilities after cochlear implantation: the effect of age at implantation on outcomes at 5 and 7 years after implantation. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 72(10):1471–1478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2008.06.016

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Harris MY, Terlektsi E (2011) Reading and spelling abilities of deaf adolescents with cochlear implants and hearing aids. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 16:24–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enq031

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Rezaei M, Rashedi V, Morasae EK (2016) Reading skills in Persian deaf children with cochlear implants and hearing aids. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 89:1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2016.07.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Geers A, Tobey E, Moog J, Brenner C (2008) Long-term outcomes of cochlear implantation in the preschool years: from elementary grades to high school. Int J Audiol 47:S21–S30. https://doi.org/10.1080/14992020802339167

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bouton S, Colé P, Serniclaes W, Duncan L, Giraud A (2015) Atypical phonological processing impairs written word recognition in children with cochlear implants. Lang Cogn Neurosci 30(6):684–699. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.1002796

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Geers AE (2003) Predictors of reading skill development in children with early cochlear implantation. Ear Hear 24(1):59S–68S. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.AUD.0000051690.43989.5D

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Marschark M, Rhoten C, Fabich M (2007) Effects of cochlear implants on children’s reading and academic achievement. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 12:269–282. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm013

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Spencer LJ, Oleson JJ (2008) Early listening and speaking skills predict later reading proficiency in pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear Hear 29(2):270–280. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.aud.0000305158.84403.f7

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Sarant JZ, Harris DC, Bennet LA (2015) Academic outcomes for school-aged children with severe-profound hearing loss and early unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants. J Speech Lang Hear Res 58(3):1017–1032. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0075

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Dillon C, de Jong K, Pison D (2011) Phonological awareness, reading skills, and vocabulary knowledge in children who use cochlear implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 17(2):205–226. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enr043

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Magee P (2014) Challenges with literacy development in children who are deaf or hard of hearing. For Res Pap 509 https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/gs_rp/509

  18. Geers A (2002) Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 33(3):172–183. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2002/015)

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Connor CM, Zwolan TA (2004) Examining multiple sources of influence on the reading comprehension skills of children who use cochlear implants. J Speech Lang Hear Res 47:509–526. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/040)

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Venail F, Vieu A, Artieres F, Mondain M, Uziel A (2010) Educational and employment achievements in prelingually deaf children who receive cochlear implants. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 136(4):366–372. https://doi.org/10.1001/archoto.2010.31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Melika L (1998) Stanford Binnet Intelligence Scale (4th Arabic version), 2nd edn. Victor Kiorlos Publishing, Cairo

    Google Scholar 

  22. Abou Hassiba A, El-Sady S, El-shobary A, Gamal El-din N, Ibrahiem A, Oweys A (2011) Standardization, translation and modification of the Preschool Language Scale-4 [Unpublished MD thesis]. Ain Shams University, Faculty of Medicine

    Google Scholar 

  23. El-Sady S, El-Shoubary A, El-Assal N, Abou-Elsaad T, Afsah O (2011) Development of a screening test battery for assessing phonological awareness in Arabic speaking children in the early elementary grades. Ain Shams Med J 62(1, 2 & 3):95–103

    Google Scholar 

  24. Khaled DM, El-Sady SR, Bassiouny SE, El Dorry GF, Hegazi MA, Hafez NG (2014) Development of an Arabic reading screening test for ages 5-9 years, Ain Shams Med. J. 65:203–209

    Google Scholar 

  25. Soleymani Z, Mahmoodabadi N, Nouri M (2016) Language skills and phonological awareness in children with cochlear implants and normal hearing. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 83:16–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2016.01.013

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Wang Y, Sibaii F, Lee K, Gill M, Hatch J (2021) Meta analytic findings on lower reading skills in children with cochlear implants medRxiv:1–83. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.03.02.21252684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mayer C, Trezek B (2015) Early literacy development in deaf children. Oxford University Press, New York, NY

    Book  Google Scholar 

  28. Mayer C, Trezek B (2018) Literacy outcomes in deaf students with cochlear implants: current state of the knowledge. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 23(1):1–16. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enx043

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cárnio M, Vosgrau J, Soares A (2017) The role of phonological awareness in reading comprehension. Rev CEFAC 19(5):590–600. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201719518316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Lepola J, Lynch J, Kiuru N, Laakkonen E, Niemi P (2016) Early oral language comprehension, task orientation, and foundational reading skills as predictors of grade 3 reading comprehension. Read Res Q 51(4):373–390. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Miller EM, Lederberg AR, Easterbrooks SR (2013) Phonological awareness: explicit instruction for young deaf and head-of-hearing children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 18:206–227. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ens067

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Bochner JH, Kelstone A (2016) Phonological knowledge and the development of language and literacy skills in deaf learners. In: Marschark M, Spencer PE (eds) The Oxford handbook of deaf studies in language. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 393–406

    Google Scholar 

  33. Mayberry RI, del Giudice AA, Lieberman AM (2011) Reading achievement in relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: a meta-analysis. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 16:164–188. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enq049

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Cupples L, Ching T, Crowe K, Day J, Seeto M (2014) Predictors of early reading skill in 5-year-old children with hearing loss who use spoken language. Read Res Q 49(1):85–104. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.60

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Harris M, Terlektsi E, Elizabeth F (2017) Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading for deaf and hearing children in primary school. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 22(2):233–242. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enw101

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Nittrouer S, Muir M, Tietgens K, Moberly A, Lowenstein J (2018) Development of phonological, lexical, and syntactic abilities in children with cochlear implants across the elementary grades. J Speech Lang Hear Res 61(10):2561–2577. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-H-18-0047

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Geers AE (2006) Factors influencing spoken language outcomes in children following early cochlear implantation. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 64:50–65. https://doi.org/10.1159/000094644

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Wass M, Anmyr L, Lyxell B, Östlund E, Karltorp E, Löfkvist U (2019) Predictors of reading comprehension in children with cochlear implants. Front Psychol 10:2155. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02155

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Yalcinkaya F (2015) Why the left ear must be chosen for implant in children to learn the mother language. IJBCS 4(2):11–20

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

No funding was obtained.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

E.H. came up with the idea and interpreted the results; S.T was responsible for the data collection and analysis; M.S. interpreted the results, wrote the manuscript and contacted the journal; and N.G. supervised the whole work and revised the writing. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mariam S. Shadi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee (REC), Faculty of Medicine, Ain Shams University; approval code (FMASU MS 21/2020). The parents of the recruited children gave their informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Tables S1–S5

. Table S1. Correlation between the ARST results (sub-tests and total score) of the study participants and their PAT results (sub-tests and total score). Table S2. Correlation between the study participants’ PAT results and their age at the time of cochlear implantation. Table S3. Correlation between the study participants’ ARST results and their age at the time of cochlear implantation. Table S4. Correlation between the study participants’ PAT results and their language ages. Table S5. Correlation between the study participants’ ARST results and their language ages.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shadi, M.S., Hafez, N.G., Taha, S.A. et al. Phonological awareness and reading abilities in elementary-school students with severe-to-profound prelingual hearing loss and unilateral cochlear implants. Egypt J Otolaryngol 38, 102 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-022-00291-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-022-00291-1

Keywords