Skip to main content

Auditory working memory measures in children with hearing impairment: a systematic review

Abstract

Auditory working memory (AWM) is the process by which information is held in the brain for a brief duration of time until either it is employed to complete a task, deleted after a short period, or transferred to long-term memory. AWM deficits have been noticed even in children with milder hearing impairments. It is essential to incorporate AWM assessment as a part of the standard audiological battery to minimize the detrimental effects of working memory deficits. The present study systematically reviews the articles published between 2011–2021 regarding test tools available to assess AWM in children with hearing impairment and the efficiency of the same. An overview of the auditory working measures such as the forward and backward digit span test; digit span subtests of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; non-word repetition; Illinois test of Psycholinguistic Skills-Forward Digit Span; Numbers reversed subtest from Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities; and Word and non-word recall subtests of Working Memory Test Battery-Children; Number recall, and Word order task from Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children II are provided in detail. The present systematic review also provides an overview of the efficiency of the assessment tools by discussing the correlation between the findings obtained in memory tasks with other auditory, verbal, and visual measures. The working memory performance in children with hearing impairment using a hearing aid or cochlear implant has been found to be affected but varies in nature depending on the degree of hearing loss.

Background

Auditory working memory (AWM) is a process in which an auditory stimulus will be stored in the brain for a brief duration in the absence of the stimulus and used to execute tasks [14]. Encoding-information processing and loading them into the memory storage,maintenance-the active rehearsal and retention of this knowledge for use in the future; and retrieval-the recall or use of the information that was stored, are the three phases of working memory [11].

WM is required for various cognitive functions, including learning, reading, and comprehension [2]. It is also thought to be a good predictor of successful communication and success in school [5]. Normal hearing (NH) children and children with hearing impairment (HI) exhibit variances in the domain of auditory experience in terms of quality and the quantity of the acoustic information affected in children with hearing impairment. This difference might impact the cognitive and linguistic development of HI children [16]. Studies state that the neural networks involved in specific aspects of cognition are affected due to auditory deprivation caused by hearing loss that remains untreated [14, 21]. Since the resources needed for higher-level understanding, such as the ability to retain auditory information in memory, must be employed for effectively decoding and interpreting the speech signal, even mild hearing loss may result in decreased performance in related cognitive activities [6, 7, 9]. Also, any changes to WM could propagate to the information processing system and can impact learning and reading as well as other cognitive activities and the distribution of attentional resources [8].

AWM abilities are often overlooked, undermining audiological assessment’s efficacy in individuals with hearing impairment (HI). Various tests developed in the recent past to assess this ability have to be included in the test battery to prevent long-term deficits. The current study aims to review the significant studies conducted on tests to assess AWM abilities in children with HI.

Methods

The systemic review was conducted based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-analyses statement (PRISMA Statement) [13]. A systematic literature search was carried out for peer-reviewed articles published from 2011 to 2021.

Information sources

PubMed/Medline, Google Scholar, and Science Direct were extensively searched for studies on AWM measures in children with HI. Lists of references and citations were searched manually for further relevant studies.

Search strategy

The search was carried out using key terms, related search phrases, derivatives, and MeSH words relevant to the study combined with Boolean operators.

“Working memory” OR “Auditory working memory” OR “Verbal working memory" AND “Assessment” OR “Measures” OR “Recall tests” OR “Digit Span Test” OR “Word Repetition test” OR “Non-word repetition test” OR “Test battery” AND “Children” NOT Auditory Processing Disorders NOT Co-morbid conditions NOT “Adults” were used as the key terms for searching studies.

Study selection

The specific inclusion criteria and exclusion criteria for the selection of studies were as follows:

Inclusion criteria

Original articles with human participants, appropriate samples (minimum sample size of 6 was considered), assessment approaches, and statistics; articles focusing majorly on the assessment of auditory WM (Working Memory); articles focusing on individuals with HI with or without hearing aid/cochlear implants were included.

Exclusion criteria

Articles published in languages other than English with poor methodology; articles on evaluation of those with auditory processing disorders or those with additional co-morbid problems; Editorials, letters to the editor, and case reports were all excluded.

Data extraction

The Rayyan QCRI (Qatar Computing Research Institute) and Mendeley desktop reference manager systems were used to integrate the search results, and the duplicate studies were removed. The studies that met the inclusion criteria were identified by screening the titles and abstracts retrieved from the search strategies. Later, the full text of the potential studies was retrieved and matched to see the eligibility. The extracted data included the article title, author details with their affiliation, year of publication, research design, study population, sample size, age group, comparison group, method of outcome measures, and keywords specific to assessing WM in children.

Quality assessment

The Critical Appraisal Skills Programme 2018 [4] was utilized to evaluate the listed papers’ methodological quality.

Results

A total of 17,600 articles were identified using database searches, with 14 duplicates eliminated. 17,586 articles were included in the title/ abstract screening. Following the title and abstract review, 27 articles were selected for the full-length article screening. Ten articles matched the inclusion criteria in the study. The remaining 17 articles were excluded mainly because of the study design and irrelevant study population. A detailed Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow chart for the selection of the study is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flowchart for the selection process of articles included in the review

Study characteristics

The study population included children with HI with or without hearing aids and CI (Cochlear Implants). 8/10 studies included participants using CI, while the remaining 2 studies included children using hearing aids. Participants with co-morbid conditions and poor performance in IQ tests were excluded from all the studies. The selected articles assessed AWM abilities directly or using verbal WM in audition mode. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III in 2 studies, Forward Digit Span and Backward Digit Span in 4 studies, ITPA-FDS in 1 study, non-word repetition in 2 studies, Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities in 1 study, Working Memory Test Battery for children in 2 studies and Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children II in 1 study were used to assess AWM. Typically developing normal-hearing children were selected as the control group in 6/10 studies and 4/10 of the included studies had no control group. Detailed characteristics of the articles included in this study are included in Table 1.

Table 1 Study characteristics of the selected articles

Quality assessment

‘The Critical Appraisals Skills Programme’ for diagnostic test study (CASP) was used to assess the quality of the studies. It consists of 12 questions to assess the article across each section to reduce bias. On analysis, it was found that all the studies were of good quality. The research questions were addressed, and there was a comparison with the reference standard in all the studies. The status of the test population was provided in detail in all studies included. All the patients received the diagnostic and reference standard tests in s10/10 studies. The test procedure was explained in detail in 9/10 studies. The results of 9/10 studies are explained so they can be calculated and worked out. The sensitivity and specificity of the tests were not provided in any of the studies, while the confidence limits have been provided in 8/10 studies for individual tasks.

Discussion

Assessment procedures

Wechsler intelligence scale for children-III

WISC-III measures cognitive functioning and was designed for children aged 6–16 years. The digit span subtest involves the child repeating progressively larger lists of digits (Wechsler 1991). Harris et al. [10] used Digit Span Forward (FDS) and Digit Span Backward (BDS) tasks. The forward task requires participants to repeat a list of random digits starting with a two-digit sequence ranging from 1 to 9. Two items are to be repeated for each sequence, and if the subject repeats no less than one of them correctly in each series, the length of the sequence is increased by one digit. Except for the change in order, the backward task is the same as the forward task. AuBuchon et al. [1] used the same procedure in their study and compared raw scores with WISC-III norms.

Forward digit span and backward digit span

Javanbakht et al. [12] used forward and backward WM to assess WM. In the FDS test, the child had to repeat a series of numbers that started with 2 digits and expanded to 7 digits, in order after they were presented. The test ends on 2 incorrect series. The number of series accurately memorized is used to grade performance. The backward digit span test is administered the same way, except the individual must repeat the numbers backward. Binaural mode scoring was done.

Soleymani et al. [15] used a set of seven numbers, i.e., 3–9 for forward and 2–8 for the backward digit span test. Every set of numbers appeared twice, and there were no repeated digits in a string. Each correct response was worth one point, with the maximum score being 14. The task terminated after two failed sequences.

Stiles et al. [16] performed the FDS and BDS tests in quiet and noise conditions. The digits were presented at 65dB SPL in the auditory mode through a monitor-mounted speaker at an angle of 0° and 0.5 m distance in the quiet. In the noisy conditions, the background noise was given from two speakers at ± 110° azimuth at a 1-m distance. In noisy conditions, the stimuli were presented at + 15 dB SNR as this level was considered a good SNR for classrooms, according to ANSI (2002). Each item in a trial is worth 0.5 points. The FDS score was calculated using the formula – 2 + 0.5 * (number of correct responses), while the BDS score was calculated using the formula – 1 + 0.5*(number of correct responses).

Tao et al. [18] used digits 0–9 in auditory-only mode using an adaptive approach (1-up/1-down) to test auditory digit span recall in both the forward and backward directions. The test began with 3 digits initially, and the sequence length was increased based on the number of correct repetitions. The first two trials were adjusted by 2 digits, while the remaining by 1 digit. Twenty-five trials were carried out in each run. The digit span score is the mean score of all trials except for the first two trials.

Illinois test of psycholinguistic skills-FDS

ITPA is used for children aged 4–8 years to assess their capability to acquire and use language. Torppa et al. [19] used the FDS subtest of the ITPA to assess the WM. The analysis employed raw scores, which do not accurately reflect the number of repeated digits. The FDS was conducted using the live voice of the experimenter in a face-to-face setting.

Non-word repetition

In the present review, two studies used the non-word repetition test. Soleymani et al. [15] developed the material for the NWR test. Sixty words in Farsi were chosen and each word's one or two phonemes were altered, turning it into a non-word with no Farsi-language meaning. A team of five speech-language pathologists and five linguists were assembled to choose appropriate non-words from the list. Twenty-five non-words met the 90% threshold.

Javanbakht et al. [12] used the Persian version of the NWR test. This test contains 40 non-sense words, and each had 2, 3, or 4 syllables. Based on the repetition speed following each item, the interval between each item presentation was around 10 seconds or shorter. The child had to listen and repeat them exactly. The number of non-words repeated correctly was used to grade performance.

Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities

It is a test to assess cognitive ability and cognitive assessments in children above age 2 through adulthood. Bharadwaj et al. [3] used the task known “numbers reversed” and “AWM” tasks from this tool. The AWM range measures include the ability to reverse numbers and AWM. The numbers presented have to be repeated in reverse order. In the AWM task, the participant is presented with a list of words and numbers, and the participant reorders the information by recalling the words and then the numbers.

Working memory test battery for children

The WM of children between the ages of 5 and 15 can be tested using the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C). The word recall and non-word recall subtests measure how well the phonological loop functions. Children must recall single-syllable words in the order they were given. The method is the same for the non-word recall subtest, and the items have the same structure as the actual words. The subtests forbidmultiple attempts. In order to control for the oro-motor deficiency as a potential confounding factor, children were instructed to repeat single-syllable and CVC non-sense words. In this review, two studies used WMTB-C to assess WM.

Kaufman assessment battery for children II

KABC-II is a test employed for children between 2.5 and 12.5 years, which offers a global intelligence assessment. Bharadwaj et al. [3] used the subtests ‘Number Recall task’ to evaluate auditory STM capacity by repeating the numbers in a given order and ‘Word Order task’ to test auditory STM by touching the silhouettes of those objects when heard.

The efficiency of the measures of auditory working memory

The efficiency of the test used in the reviewed articles has been explained through the correlation of the AWM performances with auditory, visual, and verbal measures.

Stiles et al. [16] examined the vocabulary and WM in children with mild to moderately severe HL (Hearing Loss). Phonological bias and auditory advantage were found in children with HL. Children with HI could recite longer strings of the stimulus presented. This ability was found to be better in auditory modality than in visual modality in HI children suggesting the underlying mechanism for auditory advantage is actively present in them.

The articulation rate in children with HI was comparatively slower than in normal hearing(NH) children suggesting the reduced efficiency in the subvocal articulatory system in the WM. The Corsi span used to assess the visuospatial STM was similar between NH children and children with HI with high executive function. In the auditory-quiet condition, they did better on the digit span. The presence of low-pass random background noise did not impair CNH or CHL’s working memory performance in a controlled setting with highly predictable stimuli. The authors concluded that in children with HI, WM under challenging situations could be best administered with less predictable sentences and speech-like noise in the background.

Harris et al. [10] examined the influence of verbal STM and WM capacity as a factor on children’s speech and language results following cochlear implantation. This study found that verbal STM and WM capacity process assessments accurately predicted long-term speech and language outcomes following cochlear implantation. Compared to normative data the maturation speed is slower than that of verbal STM/WM. In the long term, the baseline data showed a stronger association with speech and language outcomes than the measures at further visits on the digit span. The authors necessitate the need for integrating these results with a wider range of verbal and visuospatial STM/WM measures in the future in order to provide a more complete picture of each child's capacity for information processing. This is due to the results of this study being based solely on DS as a measure of verbal STM/WM.

Soleymani et al. [15] investigated the WM in normally developed children and children using CI between 5 and 7 years and found that some WM components are impaired in CI children. CI children with early exposure to sound significantly impacted the human memory system employed for phonological information stored and retained in STM. This study indicated that children implanted later in life had lower NWR, FDS, and BDS scores suggesting that exposure to auditory input improves a child’s performance on phonological processing tasks. The absence of standardized tests apart from the NWR test is one of the limitations of this study.

Tao et al. [18] investigated the relationship between AWM and speech perception in Mandarin-speaking children with the CI. For all speech parameters (Word in sentence recognition (quiet and noise), Chinese disyllable recognition, and Chinese lexical tone recognition), CI users dramatically underperformed NH listeners regarding speech performance. The worst CI performance was for noisy sentence identification.

Despite the considerable overlap in digit span score distributions, CI individuals' FDS and BDS scores were worse than those of NH participants. Additionally, CI participants’ articulation rate was substantially slower. In both CI and NH subjects, there was a strong correlation between the FDS and BDS scores and between the articulation rate and digit span scores. However, WM tests did not significantly correlate with CI users’ ability to recognize sentences in noise.

Torppa et al. [19] used the ITPA-FDS subtest to assess AWM in children. It was found that AWM interfered with pitch perception, wherein the CI children with music exposure had comparable performance to the control group. Longer forward digit spans improved performance on both prosodic measures. F0 discrimination and FDS showed similar developmental trends.

Willis et al. [20] used the word and non-word recall subtest from WMTB-C to assess verbal memory and the odd-one-out subtest from the Automated Working Memory Assessment. Findings indicate that despite long-term usage of CI or hearing aids, the population of children with HI has extremely low spoken language outcomes. All six study participants outperformed the control group when challenged to recite lists of non-words rather than actual words. The results suggest that these children may struggle to access their lexicon and retrieve words because they had a tougher time repeating words than non-words. It has been suggested that children with HI might not have enough phonological representations in their STM, making storing and retrieving knowledge more difficult. CI users had visual memory abilities on par with those of their hearing peers. The study’s findings also highlight the requirement for specialized memory tests that take into account the evolving needs of children with HI memory impairments. The standard NWR task only draws attention to the fact that children with HI have trouble with the task itself because of their perceptual issues. Hence, the authors suggest the use of an alternative NWR test in combination with other measures of STM and WM.

AuBuchon et al. [1] used auditory(ADS), visual(VDS), and computerized (CDS-auditory digit span task using visual representation) digit span tasks. Higher receptive vocabulary, non-verbal IQ scores, and an earlier age of deafness onset were all associated with ADS-F for CI users. In contrast, CDS-Forward was strongly connected to receptive vocabulary and the length of CI use, while VDS-Forward was significantly related to receptive vocabulary. The duration of CI use is highly correlated with both backward-span tasks. Additionally, ADS-Backward and CDS-Backward correlated with non-verbal IQ and receptive vocabulary, respectively. The authors concluded that fundamental cognitive processes that underlie STM are impaired rather than problems with speech production or audibility in long-term CI users. Average delays were longer for CI users, providing them more time to take advantage of age-related memory performance increases. The authors also concluded that while forward and backward digit spans are helpful clinical instruments for evaluating CI users’ short-term and working memory capacity, these traditional actions using immediate memory are inadequate to further understand the comprehension of the fundamental memory systems taken into consideration here.

Bharadwaj et al. [3] examined auditory and visual WM and STM in children using CI. The KABC-II, WISC-IV Integrated, and the WC III COG NU were used to measure verbal knowledge, auditory and visual STM and WM, and verbal knowledge in general. The Woodcock Reading Mastery Test III was used to evaluate reading performance. The results show that CI users’ performance on the auditory STM measures was below average. This is consistent with the idea that WM ability in children with early-onset HL is modality-specific. The advantages of visual WM tests suggest the existence of modality-specific subsystems. Children with CI performed below average on reading evaluations for listening and passage comprehension. These measures were related positively to visual STM, visual WM, and auditory STM.

Talebi and Arjmandnia [17] investigated how WM and STM interact with auditory perception and speech understanding in children with CI and compared the WM and STM in children with CI and NH groups. WMTB-C was used to assess memory performance, Categories of Auditory Performance was used to assess auditory perception, and Speech Intelligibility Rating to evaluate speech production. Children using CI performed poorer than their NH peers in WM and STM. Children with and without CI had similar levels of WM and STM. The results showed a positive and substantial relationship between the WM and STM of children with CI and their auditory perception. Additionally, there was a positive association among children with CI between WM and STM and speech understandability.

Javanbakht et al. [12] compared the WM capacity as a factor influencing speech in noise performance in NH children and children using a hearing aid. This study's working memory tests were compared to the results of the speech-in-noise perception test. It became clear that there was a strong relationship between the results of WM tests and the speech-in-noise perception scores in children with HI. The test employed in this study makes it more challenging to determine the forward and backward digit span than the conventional tests because of the stimuli utilized, which are non-sensical pseudo-words with many different syllables, that involve different levels of complex cognitive and auditory processing, such as working memory, phonological processing, auditory decoding, and the executive planning unit of the motor part of speech.

Conclusion

This systematic review has described the working memory measures available and the results in children with HI. The assessment tools focused chiefly on the auditory (and verbal) WM and STM. The present study shows that the digit span task, though used to assess WM, focuses on the memory span rather than the working memory itself. Yet, it is the most commonly used form of working memory measure. Apart from the digit span tasks, word and non-word recall, number reversing, and non-word repetition are the most frequently employed measures.

The most common memory assessments, such as forward and backward digit spans fail to provide comprehensive knowledge of basic memory mechanisms. According to the present review study, the FDS task is more sensitive than the BDS task as it requires more effort to comprehend the instructions. As a result, it highlights the necessity of reviewing WM literature in search of novel experimental strategies and behavioral tasks that can more precisely detect weaknesses in phonological storage and lexical processing. Furthermore, it is necessary to describe the verbal and linguistic processing abilities used by children with HL.

Compared to the tests for detecting the FDS and BDS, the non-word repetition test is more complex as it consists of meaningless pseudo-words with various syllables. Also, using monosyllabic non-words overcomes the potential disadvantage of perceptual difficulties in the pediatric population. It is also indicated that children with severe to profound HL have poorer performance in all the working memory tasks despite being fitted with amplification devices (hearing aid/CI). In contrast, children with mild to moderately severe HL demonstrated performance similar to their peers in digit span tasks in quiet and noise conditions. As a future direction, focus on developing deficit-specific assessment measures of AWM, and the normative data for the same has to be considered.

Limitations

More number of studies included participants with cochlear implants, and very few studies had hearing aid users. Therefore, the precise information about auditory working memory outcomes in hearing aid users could not be explained.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

HI:

Hearing impairment

CI:

Cochlear implant

NH:

Normal hearing

ND:

Children with normal development

WISC-III:

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III

DSF/FDS:

Digit Span Forward

DSB/FDB:

Digit Span Backward

NWR:

Non-word repetition

WJ III COG NU:

Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities

KABC-II:

Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children II

ITPA:

Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities

CIm:

CI with musical experience

CIn:

CI without musical experience

T1:

The first measured time point

T2:

The second measured time point

WMTB-C:

Working Memory Test Battery for Children

WM:

Working memory

AWM:

Auditory working memory

STM:

Short-term memory

LP:

Low performance

HP:

High performance

BKBSIN:

Persian version of Bamford-Kowal-Bench Speech In Noise test

SNHL:

Sensorineural hearing loss

References

  1. AuBuchon AM, Pisoni DB, Kronenberger WG (2015) Short-term and working memory impairments in early-implanted, long-term cochlear implant users are independent of audibility and speech production. Ear Hear 36(6):733–737. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000189

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Baddeley A (2003) Working memory and language: an overview. J Commun Disord 36(3):189–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bharadwaj SV, Maricle D, Green L, Allman T (2015) Working memory, short-term memory and reading proficiency in school-age children with cochlear implants. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 79(10):1647–1653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.07.006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. CASP. (2018). CASP CHECKLISTS - CASP - Critical Appraisal Skills Programme: Diagnostic Study Checklist. https://casp-uk.net/casp-tools-checklists/

  5. Daneman M, Merikle PM (1996) Working memory and language comprehension: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bull Rev 3(4):422–433. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03214546

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Desjardins JL (2016) The effects of hearing aid directional microphone and noise reduction processing on listening effort in older adults with hearing loss. J Am Acad Audiol 27(1):29–41. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15030

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Desjardins JL, Doherty KA (2013) Age-related changes in listening effort for various types of masker noises. Ear Hear 34(3):261–272. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e31826d0ba4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Fry AF, Hale S (2000) Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biol Psychol 54(1–3):1–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00051-X

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gosselin PA, Gagné JP (2011) Older adults expend more listening effort than young adults recognizing speech in noise. J Speech Lang Hear Res 54(3):944–958. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0069)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Harris MS, Kronenberger WG, Gao S, Hoen HM, Miyamoto RT, Pisoni DB (2013) Verbal short-term memory development and spoken language outcomes in deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear 34(2):179–192. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0B013E318269CE50

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Heinrichs-Graham E, Walker EA, Eastman JA, Frenzel MR, McCreery RW (2022) Amount of hearing aid use impacts neural oscillatory dynamics underlying verbal working memory processing for children with hearing loss. Ear Hear 43(2):408. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000001103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Javanbakht M, Moosavi MB, Vahedi M (2021) The importance of working memory capacity for improving speech in noise comprehension in children with hearing aid. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 147(May):110774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2021.110774

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD et al (2021) The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) 372:n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71

  14. Roy RA. (2018). Auditory working memory: a comparison study in adults with normal hearing and mild to moderate hearing loss. Global J Otolaryngol. 13(3). https://doi.org/10.19080/gjo.2018.13.555862

  15. Soleymani Z, Amidfar M, Dadgar H, Jalaie S (2014) Working memory in Farsi-speaking children with normal development and cochlear implant. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 78(4):674–678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.01.035

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Stiles DJ, McGregor KK, Bentler RA (2012) Vocabulary and working memory in children fit with hearing Aids. J Speech Lang Hear Res 55(1):154–167. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0021)

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Talebi S, Arjmandnia AA. (2016). Relationship between working memory, auditory perception and speech intelligibility in cochlear implanted children of elementary school. Iran Rehabil J, 14(1), 35-42. https://doi.org/10.15412/J.IRJ.08140106

  18. Tao D, Deng R, Jiang Y, Galvin JJ, Fu QJ, Chen B. (2014). Contribution of auditory working memory to speech understanding in Mandarin-speaking cochlear implant users. PLoS One, 9(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099096

  19. Torppa R, Faulkner A, Huotilainen M, Järvikivi J, Lipsanen J, Laasonen M, Vainio M (2014) The perception of prosody and associated auditory cues in early-implanted children: The role of auditory working memory and musical activities. Int J Audiol 53(3):182–191. https://doi.org/10.3109/14992027.2013.872302

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Willis S, Goldbart J, Stansfield J (2014) The strengths and weaknesses in verbal short-term memory and visual working memory in children with hearing impairment and additional language learning difficulties. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 78(7):1107–1114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2014.04.025

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Wong PCM, Ettlinger M, Sheppard JP, Gunasekera GM, Dhar S (2010) Neuroanatomical characteristics and speech perception in noise in older adults. Ear Hear 31(4):471. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0B013E3181D709C2

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the All India Institute of Speech and Hearing, Mysuru, for letting us conduct this research in the institute. The authors also acknowledge the University of Mysuru.

Funding

The authors did not receive funding from any of the organizations for the submitted work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors confirm the following contributions to the study: MC: formulation of research study design; acquisition, analysis, and inferring the articles; drafting of manuscript. DN: formulation of research study design; revision of the manuscript; inferring of the articles and review.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Monisha Chandran.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval and consent to participate is not applicable to this study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chandran, M., Neelamegarajan, D. Auditory working memory measures in children with hearing impairment: a systematic review. Egypt J Otolaryngol 40, 35 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-024-00593-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s43163-024-00593-6

Keywords